Paul B. Baltes (1939–2006)
Back to Wisdom
Baltes, P. B., & Kunzmann, U. (2004). The two faces of wisdom: Wisdom as a general theory of knowledge and judgment about excellence in mind and virtue vs. wisdom as everyday realization in people and products. Human Development, 47, 290–299.
There are several legitimate ways of conceptualizing and studying wisdom. One is largely informed by Western philosophy and treats wisdom as an analytic theory of expert knowledge, judgment, and advice about difficult and uncertain matters of life. Another is more consistent with Asian philosophical non-secularized traditions and treats wisdom as instantiated by wise persons or their products. The second approach is always but an approximation to the analytically constructed utopia of wisdom. Wise persons are approximations to wisdom, but they are not wisdom. Ardelt's critique of our work proposes that our theoretical conception of wisdom as a body of expert knowledge in a specific subject matter is similar to 'cold' cognition. We disagree and assert that our conception of wisdom includes as antecedents, correlates, and consequences a rich spectrum of specific cognitive, emotional, motivational and social factors as well as life contexts. Our empirical research unequivocally supports this multidimensional and collaborative view.
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2008). The fascination of wisdom: Its nature, ontogeny, and function. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 56–64.
Wisdom has intrigued both scholars and laypersons since antiquity. On the one hand, its seemingly ethereal yet obvious qualities are timeless and universal. On the other hand, these same qualities are evolving and responsive to historical and cultural change. Novel societal and personal dilemmas emerge over time, and the ways and means to deal with recurring dilemmas are revisited and updated with prudence. Building on philosophical analyses of the role of theoretical and practical wisdom in good conduct and judgment about life matters, psychologists have begun to apply scientific methods to questions about the nature, function, and ontogeny of wisdom. We outline these research directions and focus on the Berlin Wisdom Paradigm, which was one of the first attempts to bring wisdom into the laboratory. Future research on wisdom would profit from interdisciplinary collaboration and creative application of new methods drawn from developmental, social, and cognitive psychology.
Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., Maercker, A., & Smith, J. (1995). People nominated as wise: A comparative study of wisdom-related knowledge. Psychology and Aging, 10, 155–166.
This study examined whether our conception of wisdom has a psychological bias, by focusing on a group of distinguished individuals nominated as being wise. The comparison groups included older clinical psychologists and highly educated old and young control groups. Wisdom-related knowledge was assessed by 2 tasks and evaluated with a set of 5 wisdom criteria. First, old wisdom nominees performed as well as clinical psychologists who in past research had shown the highest levels of performance. Second, wisdom nominees excelled in the task of existential life management and the criterion of value relativism. Third, up to age 80, older adults performed as well as younger adults. If there is a psychological bias to our conception of wisdom, this does not prevent nonpsychologists from being among the top performers.
Glück, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Using the concept of wisdom to enhance the expression of wisdom knowledge: Not the philosopher's dream but differential effects of developmental preparedness. Psychology and Aging, 21, 679–690.
In this study, the authors explored whether wisdom-related performance could be enhanced by an instruction referring to the abstract concept of wisdom ("try to give a wise response"). The authors used three levels of activation of the concept of wisdom as well as intelligence-activation and control conditions in a heterogeneous sample of three age groups (N = 318). Results showed no general effect of the wisdom-concept instructions but did show an aptitude (resource) treatment interaction: Participants high in preparedness resources associated with wisdom exhibited some gains, whereas the performance of resource-low participants actually declined after the instruction. Implications and consequences with respect to ways of enhancing the expression of wisdom-related knowledge are discussed.
Kunzmann, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Wisdom-related knowledge: Affective, motivational, and interpersonal correlates. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1104–1119.
This study investigated the connection between wisdom as a body of expert knowledge
about the meaning and conduct of life and indicators of affective, motivational,
and interpersonal functioning. Structural equation analyses showed that individuals
higher on wisdom-related knowledge reported (a) higher affective involvement
combined with lower negative and pleasant feelings, (b) a value orientation
that focused conjointly on other-enhancing values and personal growth combined
with a lesser tendency toward values revolving around a pleasurable life, and
(c) a preference for cooperative conflict management strategies combined with
a lower tendency to adopt submissive, avoidant, or dominant strategies. These
findings corroborate the theoretical notion that wisdom involves affective modulation
and complexity rather than the predominant seeking of pleasure and also a joint
motivational commitment to developing the potential of oneself and that of others.
Kunzmann, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). The psychology of wisdom: Theoretical and empirical challenges. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Jordan (Eds.), Handbook of wisdom: Psychological perspectives (pp. 110–135). New York: Cambridge University Press.
The objective of this chapter is to present psychological work on wisdom, with a focus on the Berlin Wisdom Paradigm developed by the second author and his collaborators. In doing so, the authors will discuss some of the arguably major theoretical and empirical challenges that a psychology of wisdom has to deal with. On a theoretical level, one challenge lies in defining those core elements of wisdom that set wisdom apart from other human strengths. As we have argued in the past, it is not necessarily the ingredients (specific elements or competencies) that distinguish wisdom from other competencies such as social intelligence, creativity, or practical intelligence; rather, it is the way in which these elements interact in wisdom that is unique. The result of this interaction is an integrative and holistic approach toward life's challenges and problems--an approach to the meaning and conduct of life that embraces past, present, and future dimensions of phenomena, considers contextual variation, emphasizes value tolerance, and acknowledges the uncertainties inherent in any sense-making of the past, present, or future. The authors hope to convince the reader that it is worth the effort to face the theoretical and empirical challenges involved in developing a psychology of wisdom.
Pasupathi, M., & Staudinger, U. M. (2001). Do advanced moral reasoners also show wisdom? Linking moral reasoning and wisdom-related knowledge and judgement. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 401–415.
Wisdom and morality are both discussed as developmental ideals. They
are often associated in theoretical contexts but the nature of their empirical
relation is still an open question. We hypothesised that moral reasoning
(one facet of morality), would be related to wisdom-related knowledge and
judgement, but that the two represent different facets of high level psychological
functioning. A sample of 220 adults ranging in age from 20 to 87 years
completed measures of wisdom-related performance, moral reasoning, and
a battery of cognitive and personality measures. As predicted, moral reasoning
was positively associated with wisdom-related performance, although we
also found evidence for divergent validity of these two constructs. This
association was mediated by person characteristics (e.g., personality,
intelligence, and additional measures). In addition, as predicted by a
threshold model, very high levels of wisdom-related performance were unlikely
among those with very low moral reasoning performance. In line with neo-Piagetian
conceptions, exploratory analyses suggested that higher age was associated
with higher levels of wisdom-related knowledge and judgement only for those
with high levels of moral reasoning. Results are discussed in terms of
a lifespan model of wisdom-related knowledge and judgement.
Pasupathi, M., Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Seeds of wisdom: Adolescents' knowledge and judgment about difficult life problems. Developmental Psychology, 37, 351–361.
The present study examined adolescents' wisdom-related knowledge and
judgment with a heterogeneous sample of 146 adolescents (ages 14-20 years)
and a comparison sample of 58 young adults (ages 21-37 years). Participants
responded to difficult and ill-defined life dilemmas; expert raters evaluated
these responses along 5 wisdom criteria. Our findings confirmed that in
contrast to adulthood, adolescence is a major period for normative age-graded
development in knowledge about difficult life problems. Adolescents performed
at lower levels than young adults but also demonstrated substantial age
increments in performance. As expected, adolescents' performance varied
as a function of criterion and gender. These results hold implications
for research on adolescent development and for the development of wisdom-related
knowledge and judgment.
Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1990). Wisdom-related knowledge: Age/cohort differences in responses to life planning problems. Developmental Psychology, 26, 494–505.
A research framework and method are presented in which wisdom is conceived
as expert knowledge about fundamental life matters. Five criteria of wisdom
are specified: Rich factual and procedural knowledge, life-span contextualism,
relativism, and the recognition and management of uncertainty. As illustration,
discourse about normative and nonnormative life-planning problems associated
with different life phases was examined. Verbal think-aloud protocols,
collected from 60 Ss (25-35, 40-50, and 60-81 years of age), were scored
on the wisdom criteria by trained raters. As expected, few (5%) responses
were considered wise. These wise responses were equally distributed across
age groups. In general, wisdom-related knowledge appears to be one cognitive
domain in adulthood that does not show an overall advantage to one age
or cohort group but rather reflects individual and specific life experience.
Smith, J., Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Occupational settings facilitating wisdom-related knowledge: The sample case of clinical psychologists. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 989–999.
Wisdom can be defined as expert knowledge in the fundamental pragmatics
of life. Examined here is whether clinical practice may facilitate access
to and acquisition of such knowledge. Spontaneous think-aloud responses
to 2 wisdom-related dilemmas from young (M = 32 years) and older (M = 70
years) clinicians were compared with responses obtained from other professionals.
Raters judged clinicians' responses as higher on 5 criteria of wisdom:
factual knowledge, procedural knowledge, life-span contextualism, value
relativism, and management of uncertainty. Contrary to most studies of
cognitive aging, young and older adults did not differ. Rather, each age-cohort
group received highest ratings when responding to a life dilemma matched
to their own life phase. Discussed is the application of a wisdom framework
to assessing therapeutic treatment goals and therapist interventions as
well as global changes in client's beliefs during therapy.
Staudinger, U. M. (1996). Wisdom and the social-interactive foundation of the mind. In P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), Interactive minds: Life-span perspectives on the social foundation of cognition (pp. 276–315). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
This chapter examines the social-interactive nature of wisdom , which
is defined by the importance of social interaction for (a) its cultural
evolution as well as ist ontogeny, (b) its activation and application,
and (c) its identification and orientation. Earlier work within the Berlin
wisdom paradigm has, to a certain degree, acknowledged the social-interactive
nature of wisdom with regard to its ontogenesis and its identification.
When it comes to the application and activtion of wisdom, however, people
have been studied using a traditional person-centered paradigm. Given the
social-interactive nature of wisdom, this approach cannot be considered
an optimal performance setting. On the basis of the extremely high demands
that the elicitation of wisdom puts on knowledge and skill, one might even
argue that wisdom by definition will hardly ever be found in an individual,
but rather in cultural or social-interactive products. Drawing on research
in the area of everyday problem solving and professional expertise, as
well as on the social psychology of group problem solving, a social-interactive
wisdom paradigm was developed that gives credit to both individual and
interactive cognition in their respective contributions to the activation
of wisdom-related knowledge. The results of a first empirical study carried
out within that paradigm indicated that indeed a delicate balance between
individual and interactive cognition seems to characterize supportive wiedom-related
performance settings.
Staudinger, U. M. (2001). Wisdom, psychology of. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (Vol. 24, pp. 16510–16514). Oxford: Elsevier Science.
Since the dawn of human culture, wisdom has been considered the ideal of knowledge and personal functioning. In dictionaries, wisdom is defined as "insight and knowledge about oneself and the world ... and sound judgment in the case of difficult life problems." Psychological definitions of wisdom refer to wisdom as knowledge about the human condition at its frontier, knowledge about the most difficult questions of the meaning and conduct of life, and knowledge about the uncertainties of life, about what cannot be known and how to deal with that limited knowledge. Besides lay conceptions of wisdom, wisdom is studied as a personality characteristic, as a form of postformal thinking, and as a problem solving performance when faced with difficult and uncertain matters of life. The latter approach places wisdom within a dual component model of cognitive functioning and conceptualizes wisdom as prototypical marker of the pragmatics of the mind. This means it follows a lifespan trajectory of stability rather than decline. Investigations of the ontogenesis of wisdom have demonstrated that it is not enough to grow old in order to become wise. Rather a complicated pattern of experiential, personality, and contextual characteristics need to work together for wisdom to emerge. Recent intervention studies found evidence for wisdom-related potential.
Staudinger, U. M. (2008). A psychology of wisdom: History and recent developments. Research in Human Development, 5, 107–120.
Wisdom is a phenomenon characterized by a rich cultural history and complex associations. Across cultures and history, wisdom has been discussed as the ideal of human knowledge and character. Starting from the dictionary definition of wisdom as “good judgment and advice in difficult and uncertain matters of life,” psychologists have described wisdom as the search for the moderate course between extremes, a dynamic between knowledge and doubt, a sufficient detachment from the problem at hand, and a well-balanced coordination of emotion, motivation, and thought. Within psychological research on wisdom, two broad approaches can be distinguished. One is the study of lay conceptions of wisdom, and the other is the measurement of wisdom in individuals. In the latter case a personal and a general form of wisdom needs to be distinguished. Age trajectories and antecedents of general and personal wisdom are discussed.
Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1996). Interactive minds: A facilitative setting for wisdom-related performance? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 746–762.
Two goals guided the present study: to develop ecologically relevant
and functional interactive-minds settings for the assessment of wisdom-related
performance and to investigate whether and under which conditions such
settings would facilitate higher levels of wisdom-related performance.
After baseline assessment of wisdom-related performance, a sample of 122
natural dyads (244 participants), ranging in age from 20 to 70 years, were
randomly assigned to 5 experimental conditions. These conditions varied
in the degree to which they were judged to be ecologically relevant and
functional and involved the interaction of minds. It was found that performance
settings that were judged to be ecologically relevant and functional and
provided for actual or "virtual" (internal) interaction of minds increased
wisdom-related performance levels by 1 SD. In addition, older adults profited
more than younger adults from one setting.
Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1996). Weisheit als Gegenstand psychologischer Forschung. Psychologische Rundschau, 47, 57–77.
Weisheit, ein an Bedeutung und Kulturgeschichte reicher Begriff, wird
als Gegenstand psychologischer Forschung vorgestellt. Im Zentrum steht
die Darlegung eines empirischen Paradigmas zur Erfassung weisheitsbezogenen
Wissens und Urteilens. Ähnlichkeiten und Unterschiede dieses Paradigmas
mit Analysen kulturhistorischer Weisheitsliteratur und anderen psychologischen
Zugängen zur Erforschung von Weisheit werden aufgezeigt. In dem hier
vorgestellten Paradigma wird Weisheit definiert als Expertentum in der
fundamentalen Pragmatik des Lebens, das sich in höchstem Wissen und
höchster Urteilsfähigkeit im Umgang mit schwierigen Problemen
der Lebensplanung, Lebensgestaltung und Lebensdeutung zeigt. Fünf
weisheitsbezogene Kriterien (reiches faktisches und prozedurales Lebenswissen,
Lifespan-Kontextualismus, Wert-Relativismus, Erkennen und Umgehen mit Ungewißheit)
ermöglichen die Bestimmung der Qualität weisheitsbezogener Produkte.
In den berichteten Studien sind die bewerteten weisheitsbezogenen Produkte
Protokolle lauten Nachdenkens über schwierige Lebensprobleme, die
unter standardisierten Bedingungen erhoben wurden. Ein Modell möglicher
Vorbedingungen, Korrelate und/oder Konsequenzen weisheitsbezogenen Wissens
und Urteilens wird diskutiert. Befunde aus mehreren Studien zu Ausschnitten
dieses Modells (u.a. zur Rolle des Lebensalters, der beruflichen Erfahrung
sowie zu Leistungen von als weise Nominierten) zeigen, daß weisheitsbezogene
Leistungen reliabel zu erfassen sind, theoriekonsistente Zusammenhänge
mit Maßen der Persönlichkeit und der Intelligenz bestehen und
daß hypothesenkonforme Gruppenunterschiede in weisheitsbezogenen
Leistungen zu identifizieren sind. Schließlich werden exemplarisch
wechselseitige Querverbindungen zwischen psychologischer Weisheitsforschung
und ausgewählten anderen Gebieten psychologischer Forschung vorgestellt
Staudinger, U. M., Maciel, A., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). What predicts wisdom-related knowledge? A first look at the role of personality, intelligence, and facilitative experiential contexts. European Journal of Personality, 12, 1–17.
Wisdom has long been suggested as a desired goal of development (see
e.g. Clayton and Birren, 1980; Erikson, 1959; Hall, 1922; Staudinger and
Baltes, 1994). Questions concerning the empirical investigation of wisdom
and its ontogeny, however, are largely still open. It is suggested that
besides person characteristics, certain types of experience may facilitate
wisdom-related performance. A sample of clinical psychologists (n=36) and
highly educated control professionals (n=54) ranging in age from 25 to
82 years responded verbally to two wisdom-related tasks involving life
planning and completed a psychometric battery of intelligence and personality
measures. Three primary findings were obtained. First, training and practice
in clinical psychology was the strongest predictor of wisdom-related performance
(26%) and, in addition, showed some overlap with personality variables
in this predictive relationship. Second, 14% of the variance in wisdom-related
performance was accounted for by standard psychometric measures of personality
and intelligence. Personality variables were stronger predictors than variables
of intelligence. Important personality predictors were Openness to Experience
and a middle-range location on the Introversion-Extraversion dimension.
Third, wisdom-related performance maintained a sizable degree of measurement
independence (uniqueness). Predictive relationships were consistent with
research on naive conceptions of wisdom and our own theoretical account
of the ontogenesis of wisdom-related performance.
Staudinger, U. M., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1992). Wisdom-related knowledge in a life review task: Age differences and the role of professional specialization. Psychology and Aging, 7, 271–281.
The study adopts life review as an avenue to access wisdom-related knowledge
and examines the contribution of age and type of professional specialization
to individual differences in wisdom-related knowledge. Women from 2 age
groups/cohorts (young, M = 32 years; old, M = 71 years) and different professional
specializations (human services vs. nonhuman services) were asked to think
aloud about the life review of a fictitious woman who was either young
or old. Verbal protocols were scored on 5 wisdom-related criteria: factual
and procedural knowledge about life, life-span contextualism, relativism
of values, recognition, and management of uncertainty. Three major findings
emerged. First, human-services professionals outperformed the control group.
Second, old adults performed as well as young adults. Third, for older
adults wisdom-related performance was enhanced by the match between their
own age and the age of the fictitious character.
Contact:Note. No responsibility can be taken for the content of external links.
Baltes Office
Max Planck Institute for Human Development
Lentzeallee 94
D-14195 Berlin
Germany
Phone: +49-30-82406-0
Fax: +49-30-824-9939
e-mail: sekbaltes@mpib-berlin.mpg.deLast updated 05/2008